Skip to main content
Log in

How cultural is ‘cultural neuroscience’? Some comments on an emerging research paradigm

  • Original Article
  • Published:
BioSocieties Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

‘Cultural neuroscience’ presents itself as a new research paradigm within the neurosciences that takes the mutual constitution of culture, mind and brain seriously. As such, it has already gained considerable attention both among neuroscientists and traditional cultural psychologists. A superficial look at some of the studies published by practitioners in the field, however, might leave one with the impression that cultural neuroscience has more to do with the corroboration of well-established cultural clichés than with the broadening of mind it promises to bring about. In this article, we take a closer look at the emerging field of cultural neuroscience. The first section provides an overview of cultural neuroscience both with respect to the vision put forward by its proponents and to the actual state of research. In the second section, we engage with cultural neuroscience's parent discipline, cultural psychology, to gain a better understanding of the constructs and paradigms used by cultural neuroscientists. We conclude with three constructive proposals for a ‘truly cultural cultural neuroscience’ that avoids the popular pitfalls of essentialism and reductionism.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The term is the authors’ and is used here for convenience only.

  2. See http://culturalneuroscience.isr.umich.edu/index.html; http://culturalneuroscience.isr.umich.edu/home.htm, (accessed 30 March 2012).

  3. For a discussion of this sampling bias, see Arnett (2008) and Chiao (2009).

  4. An important exception is John Cacioppo, the Nestor of social neuroscience, who (together with Haotian Zhou) has contributed a major article to the Asian Journal of Social Psychology's special issue on cultural neuroscience.

  5. The following overview of 30 original research articles (see Appendix) is based on literature quoted in cultural neuroscience articles as relevant research in cultural neuroscience. By using this ‘sample strategy’, we utilize the demarcation of the discursive field used by the emerging scientific field of cultural neuroscience itself.

  6. This is not to say, of course, that anthropologists have not made any valuable contributions to cultural neuroscience (they certainly have – cf. Seligman and Kirmayer, 2008, in addition to the articles listed above). The point is just that few transcultural neuroimaging studies so far have been expressly conducted in cooperation with anthropologists.

  7. For the first strategy, see Adams et al (2009); Adams et al (2010); Aron et al (2010); Cheon et al (2011); Chiao et al (2009b); de Greck et al (2012); Freeman et al (2009); Goh et al (2007, 2010, 2011); Goto et al (2010); Gutchess et al (2006, 2010); Hedden et al (2008); Han et al (2011); Jenkins et al (2010); Kobayashi et al (2006, 2007); Lewis et al (2008); Na and Kitayama (2011); Sui et al (2009); Zhu et al (2007); for the second strategy see Ishii et al (2010); Ray et al (2010); for the third strategy, see Chiao et al (2009a); Harada et al (2010); Lin et al (2008); Ng et al (2010); Sui and Han (2007); Sui et al (2012). (Note, however, that Adams et al, 2009 does not really fit our description above as the comparison was really between participants (both American and Japanese) who watched own-culture faces and participants (both American and Japanese) who watched other-culture faces, not between participants from different cultural backgrounds. The same holds true in principle for Adams et al (2010), but here some elements of intercultural comparison were included.)

  8. Cf. Oyserman et al (2002).

  9. The connection between views of culture and preferred methodologies is also stressed by Dov Cohen (2007, p. 205), although his analysis of the connection is slightly different from ours.

  10. Critical discussion of the concept of ethnicity that is at work here would require a separate article. For the purpose of this article, we use the term as it is used in the literature we are reviewing, without any commitment to its viability.

  11. Not every researcher talks about ‘values, beliefs and practices’, of course; we will use Chiao's trichotomy here as a convenient shorthand for the aspects of culture cultural psychologists and cultural neuroscientists purport to study.

  12. Provided, of course, they have been living in the country in question for a sufficiently long time, or share their daily life with other members of their ethnic group (there is no ambiguity in the literature concerning the essentially social transmission of cultural values, beliefs and practices).

  13. For an example from cultural neuroscience, see Ng et al (2010).

  14. These types of priming studies seem to be more popular among cultural neuroscientists than the first one (see Chiao et al, 2009a; Harada et al, 2010; Lin et al, 2008; Sui and Han, 2007).

  15. Aron et al (2010); Chiao et al (2009a, 2009b); de Greck et al (2012); Goh et al (2010); Goto et al (2010; Gutchess et al (2006, 2010); Harada et al (2010); Hedden et al (2008); Ishii et al (2010); Lewis et al (2008); Lin et al (2008); Na and Kitayama (2011); Ng et al (2010); Ray et al (2010); Sui and Han (2007); Sui et al (2009, 2012); Zhu et al (2007). The two pairs of constructs (individualism/collectivism and independence/interdependence) actually have different origins and slightly different meanings (the first being mainly associated with research conducted by Geert Hofstede, Harry Triandis and others, the second going back to Hazel Rose Markus and Shinobu Kitayama), but as most cultural psychologists tend to use them interchangeably, we put this complication aside here (cf. Hofstede, 1980; Markus and Kitayama, 1991; Triandis, 1995).

  16. Aron et al (2010); Goh et al (2007, 2010, 2011); Goto et al (2010); Jenkins et al (2010); Lewis et al (2008). For analytic and holistic cognition more generally, see Nisbett (2003); Nisbett et al (2001).

  17. Exceptions were Adams et al (2009, 2010); Freeman et al (2009); Han et al (2011); Kobayashi et al (2006, 2007). The bias is noted and discussed by cultural neuroscientists themselves (cf. Kitayama, 2010; Kitayama and Uskul, 2011).

  18. Interestingly, the research design by Zhu and colleagues is also the only one so far that has been adapted to fit the requirements of all of the three basic strategies for cultural comparison described above: Zhu and colleagues relied on the first strategy, Ray and colleagues (2010) on the second, Ng and colleagues (2010) on the third.

  19. Cf. Choudhury et al (2009); Roepstorff (2011); Roepstorff and Vogeley (2009). There is also the more general problem, noted by many critics of the ‘neuro-hype’, that neuroscientists tend to suppose the ontological primacy of the brain. As neurological reductionism is not peculiar to cultural neuroscience, however, and might even be less common there than in other neuro-disciplines (cultural neuroscientists love to talk about the ‘mutual constitution’ of culture, mind and brain, thus committing themselves, at least verbally, to an anti-reductionist agenda), we do not pursue this issue further in this article.

  20. Markus and Kitayama (1991, p. 225) have stressed that the interdependent view is not only characteristic of Asian cultures, but of African, Latin American and many Southern European cultures as well, but they have hardly ever paid attention to these cultural contexts in their own work.

  21. Similar attempts to salvage the culture concept by discarding unwelcome connotations such as boundedness, homogeneity, coherence and stability, thus effectively blocking the transformation of culture into a ‘thing’ or an ‘essence’, can be found among anthropologists, too (cf. Brumann, 1999).

  22. ‘Cultural psychology’, in the sense in which the term is used here, is distinguished by its proponents both from ‘cross-cultural psychology’ with its search for hidden universals and from ‘indigenous psychology’ (cf. Greenfield, 2000).

  23. In recent years, Markus and Kitayama (2003b) have developed a second pair of constructs closely related to their original one: that of ‘disjoint’ versus ‘conjoint’ agency.

  24. This is not to say, of course, that the concepts of ‘independence’ and ‘interdependence’ are not frequently used in an essentializing fashion by researchers working with a less sophisticated theoretical framework. As we have tried to show in the section “Independence” and “interdependence” as core constructs in cultural psychology’, though, this is not how they were originally intended.

  25. Kitayama and colleagues tend to work with refined versions of the first and second strategies, whereas both Hong and Oyserman and their respective research teams make use of priming techniques (as explained above).

  26. Cf. again the articles quoted above.

  27. This way of describing the contrast is obviously inspired by the well-known idea that the province of the humanities (or Geisteswissenschaften) is ‘understanding’, while the province of the sciences is ‘explanation’. In other words, the ‘division of labour’ between the disciplines referred to by Kitayama and Tompson (2010, p. 98) is methodological, and nothing that needs to be ‘overcome’ (cf. loc.cit.). Both approaches – the descriptive-hermeneutical and the explanatory – have their distinct advantages as well as their limits.

  28. A first step in this direction has already been taken with the development of the ‘neuro-culture interaction model’ by Kitayama and colleagues (Kitayama and Park, 2010; Kitayama and Tompson, 2010; Kitayama and Uskul, 2011).

  29. A well-known expert on social cognition, in an article about cultural neuroscience, has even claimed that culture is ‘stored in people's brains’ (Ames and Fiske, 2010, p. 72).

  30. Thomas and Thomas (1928, p. 572). Adams and Markus (2001, p. 291) draw attention to the same fact when they argue that ‘the reification of culture into group entities is often not illusory but is present in the structure of worlds that individuals inherit’.

References

  • Adams, G. and Markus, H.R. (2001) Culture as patterns: An alternative approach to the problem of reification. Culture & Psychology 7 (3): 283–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, G. and Markus, H.R. (2004) Toward a conception of culture suitable for a social psychology of culture. In: M. Schaller and C.S. Crandall (eds.) The Psychological Foundations of Culture. Boulder, CO: Net Library, pp. 335–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams Jr, R.B. et al (2009) Cross-cultural reading the mind in the eyes: An fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 (1): 97–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adams Jr, R.B. et al (2010) Culture, gaze and the neural procession of fear expressions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 340–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ambady, N. and Bharucha, J. (2009) Culture and the brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science 18 (6): 342–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ames, D.L. and Fiske, S.T. (2010) Cultural neuroscience. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 13 (2): 72–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arnett, J.J. (2008) The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American. American Psychologist 63 (7): 602–614.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aron, A. et al (2010) Temperament trait of sensory processing sensitivity moderates cultural differences in neural response. Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 219–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P., Reuter-Lorenz, P.A. and Rösler, F. (2006) Lifespan Development and the Brain: The Perspective of Biocultural Co-Constructivism. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Begley, S. (2010) West brain, East brain: What a difference culture makes. Newsweek, 18 February, http://www.newsweek.com/2010/02/17/west-brain-east-brain.html, accessed 1 March 2012.

  • Bolger, D.J., Perfetti, C.A. and Schneider, W. (2005) Cross-cultural effect on the brain revisited: Universal structures plus writing system variation. Human Brain Mapping 25 (1): 92–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R.A. and Seligman, R. (2009) Anthropology and cultural neuroscience: Creating productive intersections in parallel fields. Progress in Brain Research 178: 31–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brumann, C. (1999) Writing for culture. Current Anthropology 40 (supplement): 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cacioppo, J.T ., Berntson, G.G., Sheridan, J.F. and McClintock, M.K. (2000) Multilevel integrative analyses of human behavior: Social neuroscience and the complementing nature of social and biological approaches. Psychological Bulletin 126 (6): 829–843.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheon, B. et al (2011) Cultural Influences on neural basis of intergroup empathy. NeuroImage 57 (2): 642–650.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiao, J.Y. (2009) Cultural neuroscience: A once and future discipline. Progress in Brain Research 178: 287–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiao, J.Y. (2010) At the frontier of cultural neuroscience: Introduction to the special issue. Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 109–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiao, J.Y. (2011) Cultural neuroscience: Visualizing culture-gene influences on brain function. In: J. Decety and J.T. Cacioppo (eds.) The Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 742–762.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiao, J.Y. and Ambady, N. (2007) Cultural neuroscience: Parsing universality and diversity across levels of analysis. In: S. Kitayama and D. Cohen (eds.) Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York, London: The Guilford Press, pp. 237–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiao, J.Y. and Blizinsky, K.D. (2010) Culture-gene coevolution of individualism-collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 277 (1681): 529–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiao, J.Y. et al (2009a) Dynamic cultural influences on neural representations of the self. Journal of Cognitive Science 22 (1): 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chiao, J.Y. et al (2009b) Neural basis of individualist and collectivist views of self. Human Brain Mapping 30 (9): 2813–2820.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiao, J.Y. et al (2010) Theory and methods in cultural neuroscience. Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 356–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choudhury, S. (2010) Culturing the adolescent brain: What can neuroscience learn from anthropology? Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 159–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choudhury, S., Nagel, S.K. and Slaby, J. (2009) Critical neuroscience. Linking neuroscience and society through critical practice. BioSocieties 4 (1): 61–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clifford, J. and Marcus, G.E. (eds.) (1986) Writing Culture: The Poetics and Politics of Ethnography. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, A.B. (2009) Many forms of culture. American Psychologist 64 (3): 194–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, D. (2007) Methods in cultural psychology. In: S. Kitayama and D. Cohen (eds.) Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York, London: The Guilford Press, pp. 196–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1996) Cultural Psychology: A Once and Future Discipline. Cambridge, MA, London: Belknap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cromby, J. (2007) Integrating social science with neuroscience: Potentials and problems. BioSocieties 2 (2): 149–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Greck, M. et al (2012) Culture modulates brain activity during empathy with anger. NeuroImage 59 (3): 2871–2882.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domínguez, D.J.F., Lewis, E.D., Turner, R. and Egan, G.F. (2009) The brain in culture and culture in the brain. A review of core issues in neuroanthropology. Progress in Brain Research 178: 43–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Domínguez, D.J.F., Turner, R., Lewis, E.D. and Egan, G.F. (2010) Neuroanthropology: A humanistic science for the study of the culture-brain nexus. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 138–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. (1998) The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology 6 (5): 178–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, J.B., Rule, N.O., Adams, Jr. R.B. and Ambady, N. (2009) Culture shapes a mesolimbic response to signals of dominance and subordination that associates with behavior. NeuroImage 47 (1): 353–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. (1973) The Interpretation of Cultures: Selected Essays. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. (ed.) (2000) Culture, mind, brain/brain, mind, culture. In: Available Light: Anthropological Reflections on Philosophical Topics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, pp. 203–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goh, J.O. et al (2007) Age and culture modulate object processing and object-scene binding in the ventral visual area. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 7 (1): 44–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goh, J.O. et al (2010) Culture differences in neural processing of faces and houses in the ventral visual cortex. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 227–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goh, J.O., Hebrank, A.C., Sutton, B.P., Chee, M.W., Sim, S.K. and Park, D.C. (2011) Culture-related differences in default network activity during visuo-spatial judgments. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, Advance Access published 22 November (doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr077).

  • Goto, S.G., Ando, Y., Huang, C., Yee, A. and Lewis, R.S. (2010) Cultural differences in the visual processing of meaning: Detecting incongruities between background and foreground objects using the N400. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 242–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P. (2000) Three approaches to the psychology of culture: Where do they come from? Where can they go? Asian Journal of Social Psychology 3 (3): 223–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gutchess, A.H., Welsh, R.C., Boduroğlu, A. and Park, D.C. (2006) Cultural differences in neural function associated with object processing. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 6: 102–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gutchess, A.H., Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A. and Gabrieli, J.D.E. (2010) Neural differences in the processing of semantic relationships across cultures. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 254–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Han, S. and Northoff, G. (2008) Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: A transcultural neuroimaging approach. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9 (8): 646–654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Han, S. and Pöppel, E. (eds.) (2011) Culture and Neural Frames of Cognition and Communication. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Han, S., Mao, L., Qin, J., Friederici, A.D. and Ge, J. (2011) Functional roles and cultural modulations of the medial prefrontal and parietal activity associated with causal attribution. Neuropsychologia 49 (1): 83–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harada, T., Li, Z. and Chiao, J.Y. (2010) Differential dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal representations of the implicit self modulated by individualism and collectivism: An fMRI study. Social Neuroscience 5 (3): 257–271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heatherton, T.F., Wyland, C.L., Macrae, C.N., Demos, K.A., Denny, B.T. and Kelley, W.M. (2006) Medial prefrontal activity differentiates self from close others. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 1 (1): 18–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H.R. and Gabrieli, J.D.E. (2008) Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science 19 (1): 12–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heine, S.J. and Norenzayan, A. (2006) Toward a psychological science for a cultural species. Perspectives on Psychological Science 1 (3): 251–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H.J.M. (2001) The dialogical self: Toward a theory of personal and cultural positioning. Culture & Psychology 7: 243–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hermans, H.J.M. and Kempen, H.J.G. (1998) Moving cultures: The perilous problem of cultural dichotomies in a globalizing society. American Psychologist 53 (10): 1110–1120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede, G. (1980) Culture's Consequences: International differences in Work-Related Values. Beverley Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y.-y. (2009) A dynamic constructivist approach to culture. Moving from describing culture to explaining culture. In: R.S. Wyer, C.-y. Chiu and Y.-y. Hong (eds.) Understanding Culture: Theory, Research and Application. New York, London: Psychology Press, pp. 3–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y.-y. and Chiu, C.-y. (2001) Toward a paradigm shift: From cross-cultural differences in social cognition to social-cognitive mediation of cultural differences. Social Cognition 19 (3): 181–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y.-y., Morris, M.W., Chiu, C.-y. and Benet-Martínez, V. (2000) Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist 55 (7): 709–720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y.-y., Wan, C., No, S. and Chiu, C.-y. (2007) Multicultural identities. In: S. Kityama and D. Cohen (eds.) Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York, London: The Guilford Press, pp. 323–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ishii, K., Kobayashi, Y. and Kitayama, S. (2010) Interdependence modulates the brain response to word-voice incongruity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 307–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, L.J., Yang, Y.-J., Goh, J., Hong, Y.-Y. and Park, D.C. (2010) Cultural differences in the lateral occipital complex while viewing incongruent scenes. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 236–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S. (2002) Culture and basic psychological processes: Toward a system view of Culture. Psychological Bulletin 128 (1): 89–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S. (2010) Interview with K. S. Frenkel. URL: http://thefprorg.wordpress.com/2010/11/29/fpr-interviews-cultural-neuroscientist-shinobu-kitayama-university-of-michigan, accessed 1 March 2012.

  • Kitayama, S. and Cohen, D. (eds.) (2007) Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York, London: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S., Duffy, S. and Uchida, Y. (2007) Self as a cultural mode of being. In: S. Kitayama and D. Cohen (eds.) Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York, London: The Guilford Press, pp. 136–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S. and Imada, T. (2010) Implicit independence and interdependence: A cultural task analysis. In: B. Mesquita, L. Feldman Barrett and E.R. Smith (eds.) The Mind in Context. New York, London: The Guilford Press, pp. 174–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S. and Park, J. (2010) Cultural neuroscience of the self: Understanding the social grounding of the brain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 111–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S. and Tompson, S. (2010) Envisioning the future of cultural neuroscience. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 13 (2): 92–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S. and Uskul, A. (2011) Culture, mind, and the brain: Current evidence and future directions. Annual Review of Psychology 62: 419–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S., Park, H., Sevincer, A.T., Karasawa, M. and Uskul, A.K. (2009) A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: Comparing North America, Western Europe, and East Asia. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 97 (2): 236–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kobayashi, C., Glover, G.H. and Temple, E. (2006) Cultural and linguistic influence on neural bases of ‘Theory of Mind’: An fMRI study with Japanese bilinguals. Brain and Language 98 (2): 210–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kobayashi, C., Glover, G.H. and Temple, E. (2007) Cultural and linguistic effects on neural bases of ‘Theory of Mind’ in American and Japanese children. Brain Research 1164: 95–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kroeber, A.L. and Kluckhohn, C.K. (1952) Culture: A Critical Review of Concepts and Definitions. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K.N., Odling-Smee, J. and Myles, S. (2010) How culture shaped the human genome: Bringing genetics and the human sciences together. Nature Reviews Genetics 11 (2): 137–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, R.S., Goto, S.G. and Kong, L.L. (2008) Culture and context: East Asian American and European American differences in P3 event-related potentials and self-construal. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 34 (5): 623–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, S.-C. (2007) Biocultural co-construction of developmental plasticity across the lifespan. In: S. Kitayama and D. Cohen (eds.) Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York, London: The Guilford Press, pp. 528–544.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, Z., Lin, Y. and Han, S. (2008) Self-construal priming modulates visual activity underlying global/local perception. Biological Psychology 77 (1): 93–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Losin, E.A.R., Dapretto, M. and Iacoboni, M. (2010) Culture and neuroscience: Additive or synergistic? Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 148–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H.R. and Hamedani, M.G. (2007) Sociocultural psychology: The dynamic interdependence among self systems and social systems. In: S. Kitayama and D. Cohen (eds.) Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York, London: The Guilford Press, pp. 3–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H.R. and Kitayama, S. (1991) Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Review 98 (2): 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H.R. and Kitayama, S. (2003a) Culture, self, and the reality of the social. Psychological Inquiry 14 (3–4): 277–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H.R. and Kitayama, S. (2003b) Models of agency: Sociocultural diversity in the construction of agency. In: V. Murphy-Berman and J.J. Berman (eds.) Cross-Cultural Differences in Perspectives on the Self. Lincoln, NE, London: Nebraska University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H.R. and Kitayama, S. (2010) Culture and selves: A cycle of mutual constitution. In: Perspectives on Psychological Science 5 (4): 420–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D. (1999) Culture and self: An empirical assessment of Markus and Kitayama's theory of independent and interdependent self-construals. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 2 (3): 289–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D. and Jones, C.A.L. (2008) Ethical issues in cross-cultural psychology. In: D.E. Mertens and P.E. Ginsberg (eds.) The Handbook of Social Research Ethics. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, pp. 323–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D. and Yoo, S.H. (2006) Toward a new generation of cross-cultural research. Perspectives on Psychological Science 1 (3): 234–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J.G. (1984) Bringing culture to basic psychological theory – beyond individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin 128 (1): 97–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J.G. (1999) Cultural psychology: Implications for basic psychological theory. Psychological Science 10 (2): 85–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J.G. (2002) Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46 (5): 961–978.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, S.J., Demorest, S.M., Aylward, E.H., Cramer, S.C. and Maravilla, K.R. (2003) fMRI investigation of cross-cultural music comprehension. NeuroImage 20 (1): 378–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Na, J. and Kitayama, S. (2011) Spontaneous trait inference is culture-specific: Behavioral and neural evidence. Psychological Science 22 (8): 1025–1032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nan, Y., Knösche, T.R. and Friederici, A.D. (2006) The perception of musical phrase structure: A cross-cultural ERP study. Brain Research 1094: 179–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ng, S.H., Han, S., Mao, L. and Lai, J.C.L. (2010) Dynamic bicultural brains: fMRI study of their flexible neural representation of self and significant others in response to culture primes. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 13 (2): 83–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R.E. (2003) The Geography of Thought: How Asians and Westerners Think Differently … and Why. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R.E., Choi, I., Peng and Norenzayan, A. (2001) Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review 108 (2): 191–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ochsner, K.N. and Lieberman, M.D. (2001) The emergence of social cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist 56 (9): 717–734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D. (2011) Culture as situated cognition: Cultural mindsets, cultural fluency, and meaning making. In: European Review of Social Psychology 22 (1): 164–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D., Coon, H.M. and Kemmelmeier, M. (2002) Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin 128 (1): 3–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D. and Lee, S.W.S. (2007) Priming ‘culture’: Culture as situated cognition. In: S. Kitayama and D. Cohen (eds.) Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York, London: The Guilford Press, pp. 255–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D. and Lee, S.W.S. (2008) Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin 134 (2): 311–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D. and Sorensen, N. (2009) Understanding cultural syndrome effects on what and how we think. A situated cognition model. In: R.S. Wyer, C.-y. Chiu and Y.-y. Hong (eds.) Understanding Culture: Theory, Research and Application. New York, London: Psychology Press, pp. 25–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, D.C. and Huang, C.-M. (2010) Culture wires the brain: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science 5 (4): 391–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulesu, E. et al (2000) A cultural effect on brain function. Nature Neuroscience 3 (1): 91–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ray, R.D. et al (2010) Interdependent self-construal and neural representations of self and mother. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 318–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roepstorff, A. (2011) Culture: A site of relativist energy in the cognitive sciences. Common Knowledge 17 (1): 37–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roepstorff, A., Niewöhner, J. and Beck, S. (2010) Enculturing brains through patterned practices. Neural Networks 23 (8–9): 1051–1059.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roepstorff, A. and Vogeley, K. (2009) Contextualising culture and social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Science 13 (12): 511–516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, R. and Brown, R.A. (2010) Theory and method at the intersection of anthropology and cultural neuroscience. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 130–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, R. and Kirmayer, L. (2008) Dissociative experience and cultural neuroscience. Narrative, Metaphor and mechanism. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry 32 (1): 34–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. (2001) Culture: Contemporary views. In: N.J. Smelser, J. Wright and P.B. Baltes (eds.) International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioural Sciences. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier, pp. 3151–3158.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. (2007) An anthropological perspective: The revival of culture – some premonitions and reflections. In: S. Kitayama and D. Cohen (eds.) Handbook of Cultural Psychology. New York, London: The Guilford Press, pp. 821–836.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. and LeVine, R. (1984) Culture Theory: Essays on Mind, Self, and Emotion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skrentny, J.D. (2008) Culture and race/ethnicity: Bolder, deeper, and broader. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 619 (1): 59–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sui, J. and Han, S. (2007) Self-construal priming modulates neural substrates of self-awareness. Psychological Science 18 (10): 861–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sui, J., Liu, C.H. and Han, S. (2009) Cultural difference in neural mechanisms of self-recognition. Social Neuroscience 4 (5): 402–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sui, J., Hong, Y.-y., Liu, C.H., Humphreys, G.W. and Han, S. (2012) Dynamic cultural modulation of neural responses to one's own and friend's faces. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, Advance Access published 15 February (doi: 10.1093/scan/nss001).

  • Tang, Y.Y. et al (2006) Arithmetic processing in the brain shaped by cultures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103 (28): 10775–10780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, W.I. and Thomas, D.S. (1928) The Child in America: Behavior Problems and Programs. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H.C. (1995) Individualism and Collectivism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Way, B. and Lieberman, M. (2010) Is there a genetic contribution to cultural differences? Collectivism, individualism and genetic markers of social sensitivity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5 (2–3): 203–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, H. and Cacioppo, J. (2010) Culture and the brain: Opportunities and obstacles. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 13 (2): 59–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, Y. and Han, S. (2008) Cultural differences in the self: From philosophy to psychology and neuroscience. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 2 (5): 1799–1811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, Y., Zhang, L., Fan, J. and Han, S. (2007) Neural basis of cultural influence on self-representation. NeuroImage 34 (3): 1310–1316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

For many suggestions and comments we thank the members of the research project ‘Cultural Neuroscience: Neural processes, social interaction and societal conflicts’ in Marburg, especially Peter Dabrock, Benno Hafeneger, Tilo Kircher and Ulrich Wagner, as well as Gerd Richter and the anonymous referees of BioSocieties. Research leading to this article has been funded by the LOEWE initiative ‘Cultural Neuroscience’ of the State of Hesse.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Appendix

Appendix

Original Research Papers Included in our Overview of ‘Current Research in Cultural Neuroscience’

Adams Jr, R.B. et al (2009) Cross-cultural reading the mind in the eyes: An fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22(1): 97–108.

Adams Jr, R.B. et al (2010) Culture, gaze and the neural procession of fear expressions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5(2–3): 340–348.

Aron, A. et al (2010) Temperament trait of sensory processing sensitivity moderates cultural differences in neural response. Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5(2–3): 219–226.

Cheon, B. et al (2011) Cultural Influences on neural basis of intergroup empathy. NeuroImage 57(2): 642–650.

Chiao, J.Y. et al (2009a) Dynamic cultural influences on neural representations of the self. Journal of Cognitive Science 22(1): 1–11.

Chiao, J.Y. et al (2009b) Neural basis of individualist and collectivist views of self. Human Brain Mapping 30(9): 2813–2820.

de Greck, M. et al (2012) Culture modulates brain activity during empathy with anger. NeuroImage 59(3): 2871–2882.

Freeman, J.B. et al (2009) Culture shapes a mesolimbic response to signals of dominance and subordination that associates with behavior. NeuroImage 47(1): 353–359.

Goh, J.O. et al (2007) Age and culture modulate object processing and object-scene binding in the ventral visual area. Cognitive, Affective, Behavioral Neuroscience 7(1): 44–52.

Goh, J.O. et al (2010) Culture differences in neural processing of faces and houses in the ventral visual cortex. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5(2–3): 227–235.

Goh, J.O. et al (2011) Culture-related differences in default network activity during visuo-spatial judgments. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, Advance Access published 22 November 2011 (doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr077).

Goto, S.G. et al (2010) Cultural differences in the visual processing of meaning: Detecting incongruities between background and foreground objects using the N400. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5(2–3): 242–253.

Gutchess, A.H. et al (2006) Cultural differences in neural function associated with object processing. Cognitive, Affective, Behavioral Neuroscience 6: 102–109.

Gutchess, A.H. et al (2010) Neural differences in the processing of semantic relationships across cultures. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5(2–3): 254–263.

Han, S. et al (2011) Functional roles and cultural modulations of the medial prefrontal and parietal activity associated with causal attribution. Neuropsychologia 49(1): 83–91.

Harada, T., Li, Z., & Chiao, J.Y. (2010) Differential dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal representations of the implicit self modulated by individualism and collectivism: An fMRI study. Social Neuroscience 5(3): 257–271.

Hedden, T. et al (2008) Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science 19(1): 12–17.

Ishii, K., Kobayashi, Y., & Kitayama, S. (2010) Interdependence modulates the brain response to word-voice incongruity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5(2–3): 307–317.

Jenkins, L.J. et al (2010) Cultural differences in the lateral occipital complex while viewing incongruent scenes. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5(2–3): 236–241.

Kobayashi, C., Glover, G.H., & Temple, E. (2006) Cultural and linguistic influence on neural bases of ‘Theory of Mind’: An fMRI study with Japanese bilinguals. Brain and Language 98(2), 210–220.

Kobayashi, C., Glover, G.H., & Temple, E. (2007) Cultural and linguistic effects on neural bases of ‘Theory of Mind’ in American and Japanese children. Brain Research 1164: 95–107.

Lewis, R.S., Goto, S.G., & Kong, L.L. (2008) Culture and context: East Asian American and European American differences in P3 event-related potentials and self-construal. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 34(5): 623–634.

Lin, Z., Lin, Y., & Han, S. (2008) Self-construal priming modulates visual activity underlying global/local perception. Biological Psychology 77(1): 93–97.

Na, J. and Kitayama, S. (2011) Spontaneous trait inference is culture-specific: Behavioral and neural evidence. Psychological Science 22(8): 1025–1032.

Ng, S.H. et al (2010) Dynamic bicultural brains: fMRI study of their flexible neural representation of self and significant others in response to culture primes. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 13(2): 83–91.

Ray, R.D. et al (2010) Interdependent self-construal and neural representations of self and mother. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5(2–3): 318–323.

Sui, J. & Han, S. (2007) Self-construal priming modulates neural substrates of self-awareness. Psychological Science 18(10): 861–866.

Sui, J., Liu, C.H., & Han, S. (2009) Cultural difference in neural mechanisms of self-recognition. Social Neuroscience 4(5): 402–411.

Sui, J. et al (2012) Dynamic cultural modulation of neural responses to one's own and friend's faces. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, Advance Access published 15 February (doi: 10.1093/scan/nss001).

Zhu, Y. et al (2007) Neural basis of cultural influence on self-representation. NeuroImage 34(3): 1310–1316.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Denkhaus, R., Bös, M. How cultural is ‘cultural neuroscience’? Some comments on an emerging research paradigm. BioSocieties 7, 433–458 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1057/biosoc.2012.30

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1057/biosoc.2012.30

Keywords

Navigation