Skip to main content
Log in

Cultural intelligence: A theory-based, short form measure

  • Article
  • Published:
Journal of International Business Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article reports the development and validation of a theory-based, short form measure of cultural intelligence (SFCQ). The SFCQ captures the original theoretical intent of a multifaceted culture general form of intelligence that is related to effective intercultural interactions. The validity of the scale is established with 3526 participants in five language groups from around the world. Results provide evidence for construct and criterion-related validity of the measure, and indicate that cultural intelligence is a single latent factor reflected in three intermediate facets. In support of construct validity the measure is modestly related to but distinct from emotional intelligence and personality and correlates positively with several indicators of multicultural experience. With regard to criterion-related validity, it relates as predicted to several dimensions of intercultural effectiveness. Implications for the measurement and understanding of culture and the influence of culture on management practice are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Figure 1
Figure 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A more detailed articulation of the theory supporting this conceptualization can be found in Thomas et al. (2008).

  2. A complete report of this analysis is presented in Thomas et al. (2012).

  3. Samples 1 and 2 were recruited using the online data collection website Amazon Mechanical Turk (M-Turk). M-Turk is a relatively new website that coordinates between workers and requesters on tasks requiring human intelligence to complete. It has become popular for data collection among social scientists because it is inexpensive and rapid. Participants recruited at M-Turk are at least as representative of the US population as traditional subject pools and data obtained are at least as reliable as those obtained using traditional methods and magnitude of effects in judgment and decision making studies is not different to that obtained using traditional subject pools (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011; Paolacci, Chandler, & Ipeirotis, 2010). Participants in Sample 1 resided in the United States and participants in Sample 2 resided in India. They were paid 50 US cents for their participation. Participants in Sample 3 were invited through the connections of former part-time EMBA (Executive MBA) and Masters students from a large Business School in France and they chose to complete the survey in either English or French. Participants in Sample 4 were EMBA students in a large Australian university who volunteered to complete the survey. Participants in Sample 5 were undergraduate students in Australia and Indonesia and their respective family members; data were collected in English and Indonesian, respectively. Participants in Sample 6 were Indonesian migrants living in Australia and they chose to complete the survey in either English or Indonesian. All participants were placed in a draw to receive one iPad mini. Participants in Sample 7 were international students studying in Australia who completed the survey in English. They were paid AUD$10.00 for participation as part of a larger study. Sample 8 consisted of employees in Turkey who completed the survey online in Turkish. Participants with work experience were recruited from graduate programs and through alumni rosters at four universities and participated voluntarily. Participants in Sample 9 were alumni of a large university recruited with assistance from the Alumni Office on a voluntary basis (N=85) and customers intercepted in a large shopping mall (N=158) in Hong Kong. The shoppers were given a US$1.3 cash voucher as an incentive after completion of the questionnaire. They completed the survey in traditional Chinese. Participants in Sample 10 were students enrolled in Masters level courses in Europe, Japan, and China who completed the survey in English. European participants were enrolled in the CEMS (formerly the Community of European Management Schools) Master in International Management Program at several European Universities; Chinese and Japanese participants were in similar programs in China and Japan, respectively. Participants in Sample 11 were working professionals in France contacted through one author’s professional networks. They volunteered to complete the survey in either English or French. Participants in Sample 12 were part-time MBA students enrolled in a large southern university in the United States who volunteered to complete the survey in English in exchange for partial course credit. Participants in Sample 13 were general Masters (who have demonstrated accuracy in previous tasks) from Amazon Mechanical Turk. They resided in the United States and were paid US$1 for participation. The majority (83.9%) of participants in Sample 14 were recruited from Amazon Mechanical Turk and were paid US$2 for participation. The remainder of the participants in Sample 14 was recruited through authors’ professional networks and volunteered to complete the survey. Samples 2, 6, 8, 9, and 12 were relatively culturally homogeneous, while other samples are multicultural, represented by larger numbers of country of birth.

  4. The details of this analysis are available from the first author.

References

  • Ackerman, P. L. 1996. A theory of adult intellectual development: Process, personality, interests, and knowledge. Intelligence, 22 (2): 227–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allen, B. A., & Armour-Thomas, E. 1993. Construct validation of metacognition. The Journal of Psychology, 127 (2): 203–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ang, S., & Van Dyne, L. (Eds) 2008. Conceptualization of cultural intelligence: Definition, distinctiveness and nomological network. In, Handbook of cultural intelligence: Theory, measurement, and applications: 3–15. Armonk, NY: M. E. Sharpe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ang, S., Van Dyne, L., Koh, C., Ng, K. Y., Templer, K. J., Tay, C., & Chandrasekar, N. A. 2007. Cultural intelligence: Its measurement and effects on cultural judgment and decision making, cultural adaptation and task performance. Management and Organization Review, 3 (3): 335–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aycan, Z. 1997. Expatriate adjustment as a multifaceted phenomenon: Individual and organizational level predictors. International Journal of Human Resource Management, 8 (4): 435–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bagozzi, R. P., Yi, Y., & Phillips, L. W. 1991. Assessing construct validity in organizational research. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36 (3): 421–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barner-Rasmussen, W., Ehrnrooth, M., Koveshnikov, A., & Mäkelä, K. 2014. Culture and language skills as resources for boundary spanning within the MNC. Journal of International Business Studies, 45 (5): 886–905.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benet-Martínez, V., Leu, J., Lee, F., & Morris, M. W. 2002. Negotiating biculturalism: Cultural frame switching in biculturals with oppositional versus compatible cultural identities. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33 (5): 492–516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bentler, P. M. 2008. EQS 6 structural equations program manual. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binet, A., & Simon, T. 1916. The development of intelligence in children: The Binet–Simon scale. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins Company.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bollen, K. A. 1989. Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bollen, K. A., & Ting, K. F. 2000. A tetrad test for causal indicators. Psychological Methods, 5 (1): 3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bond, M. H., Leung, K., Au, A., Tong, K.-K., de Carrasquel, S. R. and Murakami, F. et al. 2004. Culture-level dimensions of social axioms and their correlates across 41 cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35 (5): 548–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brannick, M. T. 1995. Critical comments on applying covariance structure modeling. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 16 (3): 201–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brislin, R. 1981. Cross-cultural encounters: Face-to-face interaction. Elmsford, NY: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brislin, R. W. 1970. Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1 (3): 185–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. 1990. Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. 2011. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6 (1): 3–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caligiuri, P. M. 1997. Assessing expatriate success: Beyond just being there. In Z. Aycan (Ed), New approaches to employee management, expatriate management: Theory and research 117–140. Greenwich, CT: Elsevier Science/JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Canary, D., & Dainton, M. 2003. Maintaining relationships through communication: Relational, contextual, and cultural variations. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ceci, S. J. 1990. On intelligence – More or less: A bio-ecological treatise on intellectual development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, C. C., Meindl, J. R., & Hunt, R. G. 1997. Testing the effects of vertical and horizontal collectivism: A study of reward allocation preferences in China. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28 (1): 44–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. 2002. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9 (2): 233–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. 1978. Knowledge structures and memory development. In R. S. Siegler (Ed), Children’s thinking: What develops?: 73–96. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T., & VanLehn, K. A. 1991. The content of physics self-explanations. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 1 (1): 69–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltman, T., Devinney, T. M., Midgley, D. F., & Venaik, S. 2008. Formative versus reflective measurement models: Two applications of formative measurement. Journal of Business Research, 61 (12): 1250–1262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. 1955. Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52 (4): 281–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cushner, K., & Brislin, R. W. 1996. Intercultural interactions: A practical guide. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeVellis, R. F. 2012. Scale development: Theory and applications, 3rd edn. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devinney, T., Pedersen, T., & Tihanyi, L. 2010. The past, present and future of international business and management. Advances in International Management, 23: 33–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diamantopoulos, A. 2005. The C-OAR-SE procedure for scale development in marketing: A comment. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 22 (1): 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiMaggio, D. 1997. Culture and cognition. Annual Review of Sociology, 23: 263–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DiStefano, J. J., & Maznevski, M. L. 2000. Creating value with diverse teams in global management. Organizational Dynamics, 29 (1): 45–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Doz, Y., Santos, J., & Williamson, P. J. 2001. From global to metanational: How companies win in the knowledge economy. Boston, MA: Harvard Business Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Earley, P. C. 2002. Redefining interactions across cultures and organizations: Moving forward with cultural intelligence. Research in Organizational Behavior, 24: 271–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Earley, P. C. 2006. Leading cultural research in the future: A matter of paradigms and taste. Journal of International Business Studies, 37 (6): 922–931.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Earley, P. C., & Ang, S. 2003. Cultural intelligence: Individual interactions across cultures. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, J. R., & Bagozzi, R. P. 2000. On the nature and direction of relationships between constructs and measures. Psychological Methods, 5 (2): 155–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Enzle, M. E., & Schopflocher, D. 1978. Instigation of attribution processes by attributional questions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4 (4): 595–599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiedler, F. E., Mitchell, T., & Triandis, H. C. 1971. The culture assimilator: An approach to cross-cultural training. Journal of Applied Psychology, 55 (2): 95–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finn, A., & Kayande, U. 2005. How fine is C-OAR-SE? A generalizability theory perspective on Rossiter’s procedure. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 22 (1): 11–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. 1979. Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34 (10): 906–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, T. D., Major, D. A., & Davis, D. D. 2008. The interactive relationship of competitive climate and trait competitiveness with workplace attitudes, stress, and performance. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 29 (7): 899–922.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. 1985. Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. 1973. The interpretation of cultures: Selected essays. Vol. 5019, New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelfand, M., Imai, L., & Fehr, R. 2008. Thinking critically about cultural intelligence: The road ahead. In S. Ang, & L. Van Dyne (Eds), Handbook of cultural intelligence: Theory, measurement, and applications: 375–387. Armonk, NY: M. E. Sharpe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, L. R. 1992. The development of markers for the big-five factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4 (1): 26–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottfredson, L. 2002. Highly general and highly practical. In R. J. Grigorenko, & E. L. Sternberg (Eds), The general factor of intelligence: How general is it?: 331–380. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gupta, A. K., & Govindarajan, V. 2002. Cultivating a global mindset. The Academy of Management Executive, 16 (1): 116–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halbesleben, J. R. B., Wheeler, A. R., & Paustian-Underdahl, S. C. 2013. The impact of furloughs on emotional exhaustion, self-rated performance, and recovery expectations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98 (3): 492–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hipp, J. R., Bauer, D. J., & Bollen, K. A. 2005. Conducting tetrad tests of model fit and contrasts of tetradnested models: A new SAS macro. Structural Equation Modelling, 12 (1): 76–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede, G. 1980. Culture’s consequences: International differences in work related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hong, Y. Y., Morris, M., Chiu, C. Y., & Benet-Martínez, V. 2000. Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55 (7): 709–720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P. W., & Gupta, V. 2004. Culture, leadership, and organizations: The GLOBE study of 62 societies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. 1998. Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological Methods, 3 (4): 424–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. 1999. Cut off criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6 (1): 1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, J. P., Lenartowicz, T., & Apud, S. 2006. Cross-cultural competence in international business: Toward a definition and a model. Journal of International Business Studies, 37 (4): 525–543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Judge, T. A., Erez, A., Bono, J. E., & Thoresen, C. J. 2003. The core self-evaluations scale: Development of a measure. Personnel Psychology, 56 (2): 303–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelloway, E. K. 1995. Structural equation modeling in perspective. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 16 (3): 215–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirkman, B. L., Lowe, K. B., & Gibson, C. B. 2006. A quarter century of culture’s consequences: A review of empirical research incorporating Hofstede’s cultural values framework. Journal of International Business Studies, 37 (3): 285–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kluckhohn, C., & Strodtbeck, K. 1961. Variations in value orientations. Westport, CT: Greenwood.

    Google Scholar 

  • Law, K. S., Wong, C.-S., & Mobley, W. H. 1998. Toward a taxonomy of multidimensional constructs. Academy of Management Review, 23 (4): 741–755.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, O., Beechler, S., Taylor, S., & Boyacigiller, N. A. 2007. What we talk about when we talk about “global mindset”: Managerial cognition in multinational corporations. Journal of International Business Studies, 38 (2): 231–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little, T. D. 1997. Mean and covariance structures (MACS) analyses of cross-cultural data: Practical and theoretical issues. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 32 (1): 53–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Conner, A. L. 2013. Clash: 8 cultural conflicts that make us who we are. New York: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. C. 2013. Assessing cross-cultural competence: A review of available tests. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 44 (6): 849–873.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, M. 1937. Cooperation and competition among primitive peoples. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mor, S., Morris, M., & Joh, J. 2013. Identifying and training adaptive cross-cultural management skills: The crucial role of cultural metacognition. Academy of Management Learning and Education, 12 (3): 453–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, M. W., Leung, K., Ames, D., & Lickel, B. 1999. Views from inside and outside: Integrating emic and etic insights about culture and justice judgments. Academy of Management Review, 24 (4): 781–796.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuliep, J. W. 2002. Assessing the reliability and validity of the generalized ethnocentrism scale. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 31 (4): 201–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuliep, J. W., & McCroskey, J. C. 1997. The development of a US and generalized ethnocentrism scale. Communication Research Reports, 14 (4): 384–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paige, R. M. 2004. Instrumentation in intercultural training. In D. Landis, J. M. Bennett, & M. J. Bennett (Eds), Handbook of intercultural training: 85–128. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Paolacci, G., Chandler, J., & Ipeirotis, P. G. 2010. Running experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgment and Decision Making, 5 (5): 411–419.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pedhazur, E. J., & Schmelkin, L. P. 1991. Measurement, design, and analysis: An integrated approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, M. F., & Wood, R. E. 2008. Cognitive structures and processes in cross-cultural management. In P. B. Smith, M. F. Peterson, & D. C. Thomas (Eds), Handbook of cross-cultural management research: 15–34. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rohner, R. 1984. Toward a conception of culture for cross-cultural psychology. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 15 (2): 111–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rossiter, J. R. 2002. The C-OAR-SE procedure for scale development in marketing. International Journal of Research in Marketing, 19 (4): 305–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruben, B. D., & Kealey, D. J. 1979. Behavioral assessment of communication competency and the prediction of cross-cultural adaptation. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 3 (1): 15–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwab, D. P. 1980. Construct validity in organizational behavior. Research in Organizational Behavior, 2: 3–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H. 1992. Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. In M. P. Zanna (Ed), Advances in experimental social psychology: 1–65. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharma, P. 2014. Consumer ethnocentrism: Reconceptualization and cross-cultural validation. Journal of International Business Studies. advance online publication, 21 August doi: 10.1057/jibs.2014.42.

  • Shore, B. 1996. Culture in mind: Cognition, culture, and the problem of meaning. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shore, L. M., Cleveland, J. N., & Goldberg, C. B. 2003. Work attitudes and decisions as a function of manager age and employee age. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88 (3): 529–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. B., & Hecker, J. 2006. Cross-national working. Presentation to the International Association of Cross-Cultural Psychology Congress, Spetzes, Greece, July.

  • Smith, P. B., & Peterson, F. M. 1988. Leadership, organizations and culture: An event management model. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. B., Peterson, M. F., & Schwartz, S. 2002. Cultural values, sources of guidance and their relevance to managerial behavior: A 47 nation study. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 33 (1): 188–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spector, P. E. 1994. Using self-report questionnaires in OB research: A comment on the use of a controversial method. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 15 (5): 385–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. 1996. Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, G. K. 2008. The cultural dynamics of mergers and acquisitions. In P. B. Smith, M. F. Peterson, & D. C. Thomas (Eds), Handbook of cross-cultural management research: 431–448. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1985. Beyond IQ: A triarchic theory of human intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. 1997. The concept of intelligence and its role in lifelong learning. American Psychologist, 52 (10): 1030–1037.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J., Lautrey, J., & Lubart, T. I. 2003. Models of intelligence. Hyattsville, MD: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. 1989. Using multivariate statistics, 2nd edn. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tadmor, C., & Tetlock, P. E. 2006. Biculturalism: A model of the effects of second-culture exposure on acculturation and integrative complexity. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37 (2): 173–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ting, K. F. 1995. Confirmatory tetrad analysis in SAS. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2 (2): 163–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, D. C., & Fitzsimmons, S. R. 2008. Cross-cultural skills and abilities: From communication competence to cultural intelligence. In P. B. Smith, M. F. Peterson, & D. C. Thomas (Eds), The handbook of cross-cultural management research 201–215. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, D. C., & Inkson, K. 2003. Cultural intelligence: People skills for global business. San Francisco, CA: Berrett-Koehler.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, D. C., & Lazarova, M. B. 2014. Essentials of international human resource management: Managing people globally. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, D. C., Stahl, G., Ravlin, E. C., Poelmans, S., Pekerti, A., Maznevski, M., Lazarova, M. B., Elron, E., Ekelund, B. Z., Cerdin, J. L., Brislin, R., Aycan, Z., & Au, K. 2008. Cultural intelligence: Domain and assessment. International Journal of Cross-Cultural Management, 8 (2): 123–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, D. C., Stahl, G., Ravlin, E. C., Poelmans, S., Pekerti, A., Maznevski, M., Lazarova, M. B., Elron, E., Ekelund, B. Z., Cerdin, J. L., Brislin, R., Aycan, Z., & Au, K. 2012. Development of the cultural intelligence assessment. In W. H. Mobley (Ed), Advances in Global Leadership 155–178, Bingley, UK: Emerald Group.

  • Triandis, H. C. 1975. Culture training, cognitive complexity, and interpersonal attitudes. In R. W. Brislin, S. Bochner, & W. J. Lonner (Eds), Cross-cultural perspectives on learning: 39–78. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tung, R. L., & Varma, A. 2008. Expatriate selection and evaluation. In P. B. Smith, M. F. Peterson, & D. C. Thomas (Eds), Handbook of cross-cultural management research: 367–378. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dyne, L., Ang, S., & Koh, C. 2008. Development and validation of the CQS: The cultural intelligence scale. In S. Ang, & L. Van Dyne (Eds), Handbook of cultural intelligence: Theory, measurement, and applications: 16–40. Armonk, NY: M. E. Sharpe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, C., Fischer, R., Lam, F. S. Z., & Hall, L. 2009. The convergent, discriminant, and incremental validity of scores on a self-report measure of cultural intelligence. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 69 (1): 85–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. 1999. The measurement of sociocultural adaptation. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 23 (4): 659–677.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, C., & Rana-Deuba, R. 1999. Acculturation and adaptation revisited. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30 (4): 422–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. 1939. The measurement of adult intelligence. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins Company.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wong, C. S., & Law, K. S. 2002. The effects of leader and follower emotional intelligence on performance and attitude: An exploratory study. The Leadership Quarterly, 13 (3): 243–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by grants from the Social Science and Humanities Research Council of Canada and from the Australian School of Business, University of New South Wales.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David C Thomas.

Additional information

Accepted by Timothy Devinney, Guest Editor, 17 October 2014. This article has been with the authors for two revisions.

Appendices

APPENDIX A

SFCQ Scale

illustration

figure a

APPENDIX B

Measures Used in Validating SFCQ

EQ was measured by the 16-item EI scale developed by Wong and Law (2002) on a 5-point Likert scale (1=strongly disagree, 5=strongly agree). Internal consistency reliability as measured by Cronbach’s α was 0.88.

Personality was measured on the Big Five personality traits (Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, and Openness) with 50 marker items from the International Personality Item Pool (Goldberg, 1992). Each personality characteristic was measured by 10 items on a 5-point Likert scale (1=very inaccurate, 5=very accurate). Internal consistency reliability as measured by Cronbach’s α was α=0.88 for Extraversion, α=0.81 for Agreeableness, α=0.87 for Neuroticism, α=0.81 for Openness, and α=0.81 for Conscientiousness.

Intercultural effectiveness was measured by three items tapping key aspects of effectiveness in a cross-cultural context with a scale developed by Thomas et al. (2012) of specific relevance to the validation of cultural intelligence. This approach is based on research on successful adjustment to a foreign culture (Brislin, 1981; Cushner & Brislin, 1996; Ruben & Kealey, 1979) and expatriate adjustment and performance (e.g., Aycan, 1997; Tung & Varma, 2008). The literature in these areas has summarized the characteristics of effective intercultural interaction in an organizational context as (a) good personal adjustment indicated by feelings of contentment and well-being when interacting with culturally different others, (b) development and maintenance of good interpersonal relationships with culturally different others, and (c) the effective completion of task-related goals in an intercultural context. Based on this definition of intercultural effectiveness the 3-item scale was used to tapping each of the three elements. Responses were provided on a 5-point scale (1=disagree strongly, 5=agree strongly). The internal consistency reliability of the 3-item scale as measured by Cronbach’s α was 0.59. Given the breadth (high bandwidth) of the construct, we were not surprised with this level of internal consistency. Bandwidth is the amount of information obtained, while fidelity is the consistency of information. The idea of balancing bandwidth and fidelity in measures of this type has been widely discussed and has generally had broad acceptance (see, e.g., Chen, Meindl, & Hunt, 1997).

Sociocultural acculturation and adaptation was measured with two instruments in two different samples. We measured sociocultural acculturation with 20 items from the Acculturation Index (Ward & Rana-Deuba, 1999) in Sample 6 (Indonesian migrants in Australia). This scale measures how similar immigrants’ experiences and behaviors are to locals in the host country evaluated on a 7-point Likert scale (1=not at all similar, 7=very similar). Internal consistency reliability as measured by Cronbach’s α was 0.97. We used 20 items from the Sociocultural Adaptation Scale (Ward & Kennedy, 1999) in Sample 7 (international students in Australia). This scale measures the extent of difficulty people experience in a number of areas in the host culture, such as making friends and following rules, using a 5-point Likert scale (1=no difficulty, 5=extreme difficulty). Internal consistency reliability was 0.96.

Ethnocentrism was measured by the Generalized Ethnocentrism Scale developed by McCroskey and Neuliep (Neuliep, 2002; Neuliep & McCroskey, 1997). This scale contains 22 items (including 15 scoring items and 7 distractors) with a 5-point response scale (1=strongly disagree, 5=strongly agree). It measures a person’s ethnocentrism regardless of his or her cultural background. Internal consistency reliability as measured by Cronbach’s α was 0.90.

Job performance was measured by a 4-item, self-rating scale consistent with similar measures used in previous studies (see Fletcher, Major, & Davis, 2008; Halbesleben, Wheeler, & Paustian-Underdahl, 2013; Shore, Cleveland, & Goldberg, 2003). Participants indicated how they felt their supervisor would rate their (1) overall performance, (2) problem solving performance, (3) leadership effectiveness, and (4) communication effectiveness on a 4-point scale (anchored by 1=below average to 4=outstanding). Internal consistency reliability as measured by Cronbach’s α was 0.81.

Attribution accuracy was measured by participants’ responses to the behavior of a protagonist depicted in two short videos. The two videos were scripted and filmed (see Thomas et al., 2012) based on scenarios derived from a large-scale survey (Smith & Hecker, 2006) of the most frequent failures in intercultural interactions. For example, one video involved indirect vs direct communication behavior and interpersonal space differences. Prior to filming, the scripts were pretested in two multicultural focus groups. Based on these reactions and the reactions of the multicultural research team, scripts were modified to convey a sense of mundane realism (Enzle & Schopflocher, 1978). The short videos (about 2 min each) were made by a professional film company and employed professional actors. Subsequent responses to the videos by focus groups confirmed the realism of the scenarios depicted.

The four choices of response to the video were constructed in a similar manner to the development of the choices offered in the original culture assimilator (Fiedler, Mitchell, & Triandis, 1971). The correct response among the four choices was established by an international panel of experts on intercultural behavior. The accuracy of participants’ scores was the sum of correct responses ranging from 0 to 2.

Demographics: Participants reported their age, gender, education level (1=primary school, 2=some secondary (high) school, 3=secondary (high) school, 4=some post-secondary (university/college/polytechnic), 5=university degree, 6=post-graduate degree (e.g., Masters, Doctorate, LLD, MD), country of birth, whether they were born in the same country as their parents, their parents’ country of birth, number of languages they speak other than their native language (1=none, 2=one, 3=two, 4=three or more), number of countries in which they have lived (1=one country, 2=two to three different countries, 3=four to five different countries, 4=over five different countries) and visited (1=none, 2=one country, 3=two to three different countries, 4=four to five different countries, 5=over five different countries), whether they have a close friend from another culture (1=yes, 2=no), whether their best friend is from another culture (1=yes, 2=no), and whether they interact with people from other cultures at work (1=yes, 2=no).

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Thomas, D., Liao, Y., Aycan, Z. et al. Cultural intelligence: A theory-based, short form measure. J Int Bus Stud 46, 1099–1118 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1057/jibs.2014.67

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1057/jibs.2014.67

Keywords

Navigation